Masks and the Person

Masks and the Person
Published: Apr 28, 2024
Standfirst
Seeing faces where there aren’t any . . . Rosie Jackson asks the question: "How are masks and selves entwined?"
Body

 

Seeing Faces Where There Aren't Any_Rosie Jackson_journal of wild culture ©2024.jpg

'Transformation Mask' (inset).' Kwakwaka'wakw: British Columbia, Canada. [o]

 

If you have ever sat in an intensive care unit with a relative in an oxygen mask, you’ll know how inappropriately funny it is. This is partly due to facial distortion, partly due to inhibited movement, because the brain makes sense of the new geometries and extensions, translating the plastic nozzle and trailing pipe into something familiar and elephantine: an awkwardly constructed ancient beast, all wrinkly skin and pulsating veins.

Historically, masks as reproductions of a stock character, spirit, or fantasy figure have been used to incite transformation. We’ve not only come to terms with the facelessness of mask-wearing celebrities Lady Gaga, Bjork, Deadmau5 and Daft Punk — or more appropriately their many faces — we enjoy their inflated personas. Encountering them only as stadium demi-gods (or digitally, several times removed) they may as well be fictional puppets who we enjoy for not being real.

 

The ability to recognise the motivations of a potential predator, earlier rather than later, would have been beneficial to our earliest ancestors.



When real people wear masks – friends, serial killers, tights-wearing vigilantes – what is most terrifying for observers is not the mask itself (anything will do) but the fear that in wearing the mask the person has become someone or something else. While their face is hidden, they have broken their contract with us and the world. Normal rules do not apply. The dehumanising element doesn’t come from the mask itself (though it helps) but from the observation of a body without a decipherable face. Frozen-faced characters like Christiane from Eyes Without A Face, Michael Myers in Halloween, the killers in Scream can be horrific or funny and both.

Identifying faces has always been integral to our survival. In studying the benefits of our hypersensitivity to faces, astronomer and science communicator Carl Sagan noted we require only the most minimal of details to recognise faces from a distance and in poor visibility. We are also hard wired to see patterns in chaos: Jesus on a slice of toast, and the man in the moon. This phenomenon, called pareidolia, causes us to see faces where there aren’t any. A 2009 study found that the nerve activation in the brain in response to objects incidentally perceived as faces occurs at a similar time and location to that evoked by images of faces and real faces. Furthermore, repeated presentation of new visual shapes that were interpreted as meaningful led to decreased activity in response to real objects. These results indicate that interpretation of ambiguous stimuli depends on similar processes as those elicited for known objects.

 

The mask, in its replication, replacement or removal of face, provokes a perpetual double take, drawing attention to our own duplicity. Image: Daehyun Kim. [o]


This mechanism is responsive to subtle changes. People are quickly able to identify a "face," analyse the "face-like" object, and determine the emotional state and identity the subject – even before the conscious mind begins to process the information. Even the "stick figure face" – a few lines and a circle – can convey mood and intention. In his 1995 book The Demon-Haunted World, Sagan argues that this ability to recognise the motivations of a potential predator and pre-empt threatening situations with a flee-or-attack response earlier rather than later would have been beneficial to our earliest ancestors. Still, the face can be a comfort or a warning sign. The bridge between thought and action, it tells us what to do. It is the first place we look when we meet someone for the first time.

The uncanniness of masks centres on the horror film moment, the moment at which our expectation of a face is derailed, when the man shows us a tumour where his face should be, or the little girl cannot turn to face us. It is this nebulous grey area which masks occupy, the space between the reality and the expectation. The mask, in its replication, replacement or removal of face, provokes a perpetual double take, drawing attention to our own duplicity. When faced with a fake face, we imagine the real face we cannot see, what lies beneath, and what lies beneath that. The uncertainty makes us think about what it is to be both a face and a mind, what makes a person. It is a tension you can’t shake off. The “Uncanny Valley,” a term invented by robotics professor Masahiro Mori in 1970, attempts to measure this tension. The hypothesis states that as the appearance of a robot is made more human, a human observer's emotional response to the robot becomes increasingly positive and empathic until at a certain point it quickly turns to strong revulsion. However, as the robot's appearance continues to become more like a human being, the emotional response becomes positive once more and approaches human-to-human empathy levels. The area of repulsive response aroused by a robot with appearance and motion between a "barely human" and "fully human" entity is called the Uncanny Valley.

 

Seeing Faces Where There Aren't Any_journal of wild culture ©2024
'Transformation Mask' (Kwakwaka'wakw: British Columbia, Canada). Peabody Museum of Archaeology and Ethnology, Cambridge, Massachusetts, here presented in an exhibition in Paris.

 

This follows Freud’s concept of the uncanny, which describes instances and objects which are both attractively familiar but also uncomfortably strange — and our tendency to reject rather than rationalise such disconcerting events. The fact that eerily real-looking robots illicit this same response points to our dependence on human recognition and classification, but also suggest an uncertainty about our own make-up. While we can assume that other humans have similar thoughts, feelings and physical needs to ourselves, hybrid creatures are an unknown quantity, we cannot get the measure of them. Of course, it is a thorny road between humanity and hybridity. From prosthetics to pacemakers, we’re becoming very good at enhancing our identity with add-ons and modifications. We are more sympathetic to hybrids who start off human and step slowly away than those advancing in the other direction.

How we feel about our own composition is central to our well being. In 1941 American psychiatrist Hervey Cleckley explored the concept of psychopathic fake selves in his seminal book The Mask of Sanity, and in the 1970s psychoanalyst D. W. Winnicott developed Freud’s belief in the id and the ego into a simpler identity model: the “false self” and the “true self,” and sometimes simply “self.” Several of Winnicott’s patients were suffering from a sense of being empty, dead or "phoney." Despite an overriding feeling of being false to themselves, these patients continued to perform the "show of being real". For Winnicott, the false self was what allowed a person to present a "polite and mannered attitude" to society and acted as a shield for the true self. Like the character Rorschach from the DC comics Watchmen series whose mask displays a constantly morphing ink blot, our faces only show as much as people want to see in them.

It is comforting to believe in an authentic self, cocooned but actionable if summoned in the right way. That our real thoughts seem to be burbling away somewhere deep inside, while outwardly our faces grin and bear it seems to be a necessary part of existence. However in extreme cases, it can be paralysing. Sometimes diagnosed as depersonalisation disorder, what feels like a severe disconnection from one’s self also results in a divorce from physicality. Documented by Jonathan Caouette in the award-winning film Tarnation, the condition is a sort of locked-in syndrome in reverse. Sufferers are cast out from their material presence and all thoughts and sensations feel unrelated to their reality or environment. Life passes them by as if it were happening to somebody else. The result is often that they cannot locate themselves beyond the mask. Such detachment from all fixed points is disorientating but desirable for alternate reality junkies. LSD-induced trips, bending life and nature into unrecognisable proportions, bring on similar out-of-body experiences which connect users with some kind of truth, if not their "true selves," whatever that might mean.

 

Phoney Faces_Rosie Jackson_Lucien Freud painting_journal ofd wild culture

Lucian Freud, The Painter’s Mother Resting I. 1976. Oil on canvas, 90.2 x 90.2 cm. Collection Irish Museum of Modern Art. [o]


Isolated epiphanies aside, in what other more permanent ways can we capture the shifting perceptions of our insides? Artistic representations can shed light on what lies beneath. Lucian Freud’s portraits, many depicting the artist’s close friends and family, disclose invisible internal ground in their display of fleshiness. In his own words, Freud paints "a distinction between fact and truth.” Catching someone in a state of undress, de-clothed and disclosed, is rare; here the flesh, a mask of sorts, stretches and trembles when worked up to such proportions. When given this much attention, marks and body contours become signs of consciousness.

In the portraits of Freud's mother, observing her grief over the death of his father, or lying in her hospital bed, we see her emotional life has literally shaped the face, but also her introspection, and so also the relation between the intangible and the body. Of course it is still projection, but that’s about as close as any of us can get. ≈ç

 

 

ROSIE JACKSON is a psychotherapist and counsellor. In 2012 she served as the senior editor of The Journal of Wild Culture during its inception as an online publication in London. This article is a minimally modified version of the original published at that time under the title "Phoney Faces: Facial Removal and How Masks Work." Rosie lives in London.

 

 

Add new comment